Incident
commanders in missing person searches rely on lost person behavior profiles for
the initial deployment of resources and development of objectives. To
characterize the behavior of lost Alzheimer's subjects in the Middle Atlantic
States, five years of search and rescue data from Virginia has been analyzed.
William Syrotuck was the first to
systematically collect and analyze lost person behavior1. Barry
Mitchell presents several subject profiles based on a large data set collected
by the National Association for Search and Rescue.2 Mitchell's work
provides both behavior profiles and statistics to help predict the lost
subject's location. The profiles include hunters, hikers, children (by age
group), the mentally retarded, berry pickers and the elderly. These studies are
incorporated into the major textbooks and field guides used by incident
commanders.3,4 More importantly, planners use this information during
searches. Unfortunately, search subjects suffering from Alzheimer's disease have
either been grouped with elderly subjects or been
undocumented.
Early estimates of the prevalence of
Alzheimer's disease was two million cases in the United States.5
Current estimates are four million.6 The increase is believed to be
due to both an increase in awareness of the disease and an increase in medium
age of the U .S. population. Regional demographics also greatly affect the
percentage of Alzheimer's cases found in each state.
Alzheimer's disease is known as a disease
of exclusion since it can only be diagnosed positively after the subject's
death. However, Dementia of Alzheimer's Type (DAT) has been well characterized
and can be documented with behavioral tests.7-8 DAT is a chronic
progressive disorder of unknown onset in which the affected individual suffers:
Initially, these changes are difficult to
detect. However, they will eventually lead to such problems as wandering,
pacing, aggression, irritability, withdrawal, fear and anxiety.
DA T is delineated into mild, moderate
and severe categories.1O
The earliest signs of DAT often appear during trips to unfamiliar
surroundings. The patient is often visiting friends or family and becomes
confused only a short distance from the residence. The patient with moderate DAT
often appears normal even though they suffer from memory problems. Usually the
caregiver relates stories about the patient previously be- coming lost, a
decline in personal hygiene, an inability to carry out financial matters and an
inability to remember recent conversations. Those patients suffering from severe
DAT will clearly be recognized as suffering from "mental problems." Caregivers
will usually report a patient with incontinence, an inability to feed or groom
themselves, and a lack of recognition of loved ones.11 In cases of
severe DAT:
Four of these characteristics, wandering,
agitation, poor hygiene and incontinence, significantly increase with further
deterioration of the DAT patient. Among mild cases of DAT, 18% of the patients
wander, while in severe cases wandering increases to 50%.12 This
particular trait has serious consequences when the patient wanders into a
wilderness or rural location.
It is important to realize that 35% of
DAT patients have a coexisting diagnosis.13 The most common
additional problems are depression (25%), overmedication, hypothyroidism,
hyperparathyroidism, diabetes, acute infections and Parkinson's
disease.14 Most of these problems tend to decrease activity and the
potential distance a DAT patient may travel.15
DAT subjects differ significantly from
other lost subject behavioral profiles. Hikers, hunters and other groups venture
into the woods with both a purpose and equipment. Therefore, the types of clues
they leave often involve multiple physical objects. Containment is an effective
technique in searches for hikers and hunters. The tactic relies upon the lost
subject recognizing and following
features such as a road, trail or string barrier. DAT subjects may simply
wander into the woods and tend not to leave physical clues other than signs of
passage and scent. The only other potential physical clues are the subjects'
discarded clothing or pocket contents. DAT subjects may not recognize the value
of such features or even recognize the fact they are lost
The Virginia Department of Emergency
Services (DES) is responsible for coordinating search and rescue (SAR)
activities throughout the state. In 1986, a new management system was introduced
that utilizes selected operations personnel to 'handle all requests for SAR
assistance. Therefore, all SAR requests are handled by personnel involved in SAR
education and operations. Additionally, it created a new record-keeping system
and database.16 This retrospective study begins in June 1986 with the
first state recorded mission (VAOO1) and ends in June 1991 (VA234). Due to
duplications in the numbering of some missions, 245 incidents are covered.
System Description. The DES SAR duty officer is
responsible for alerting state field operational resources, coordination between
the local law enforcement agency and state police, coordination with local
emergency coordinators, coordination between state and federal resources, field
support and data collection.
State field operational resources are
tested by the independent Virginia Search and Rescue Council (VASARCO). VASARCO
has representatives from all active statewide SAR resources. State resources
include air scent dog teams, dog tracking teams, mounted horse teams, explorer
scouts, management teams, ground teams, tracking teams, the Civil Air Patrol and
government resources. The state peacetime disaster plan places responsibility
with the local law enforcement agency if the local plan does not otherwise
specify the role. The initial response from local law enforcement varies
depending upon the locality. While some law enforcement officials contact DES
immediately to request state resources, many localities will conduct search
operations for six hours to several days before requesting state help.
To activate the system, a citizen reports
the missing person to a local law enforcement agency, rescue squad or fire
department. Once a request for state assistance is made, the initial response
usually consists of an overhead management team, air scent dog teams, tracking
dog teams, hasty teams and helicopters. After assessing the situation, the
overhead team is responsible for requesting additional resources.
Criteria for Inclusion. Only searches issued a DES mission number
are included in the relationship studies and the point last seen analysis. Five
additional searches before 1986 and four additional searches after June 1991
are added to the clues, roadway crossings, techniques used, medical
conditions and attraction analysis. Mission numbers are issued only when state
SAR resources are dispatched to the incident. All data was collected from a
combination of the DES Missing Person Reports, DES after Action Reports and
Virginia SAR Council mission summaries. These reports generally are completed by
the incident commander or a general staff member. Missing information is often
collected later by the DES SAR officer. Copies of the original reports were
furnished by DES.
The state data forms do not include
information concerning the medical diagnosis of injured or dead subjects, clues
discovered during the search, techniques used to locate the subject or whether
the subject crossed any roads. Therefore, a review of state records, search team
records and personal records of the incidents was performed.
Classification as a DAT missing person is
based solely upon the caregiver's description of the subject. Incident
commanders have no specific training to allow them to determine the validity of
such claims. The data collection form has no specific question concerning a DAT
description or mental status of the search subject. Therefore, it is completely
voluntary for the compiler to fill in a DAT description in the "other pertinent
information" blank. If the compiler did not mention Alzheimer's disease,
dementia, senility or confused, the missing person was classified as either
elderly (if over 60 years of age) or placed into another category (retarded,
despondent, etc.).
Year |
Total
Searches |
DAT
Searches |
%DAT |
1987 |
33 |
1 |
3% |
1988 |
40 |
4 |
10% |
1989 |
49 |
8 |
16% |
1990 |
54 |
9 |
16% |
Data Coding. The information provided on the state
form includes: state mission number, age and sex of subject, time the
subject was last seen, date subject was last seen, type of location where last
seen (nursing home, residence, etc.), air distance from subject last seen to
where subject found, description of terrain where subject located and a brief
summary of subject's medical condition.
The information recorded from personal
records includes date, subject name, location, condition of subject,
successful or suspended mission, field diagnosis of subject's medical condition,
any verifiable clues located, terrain description of find location, whether the
subject crossed or left roads and the search technique that located the subject.
Results
Twenty-nine (12%) out of
245 recorded state incidents involved possible DAT sufferers. This particular
category was the largest in the data set. The other most prevalent search types
included suicidal ( 12% ) , children ( 11% ) , hikers (10%), drownings (9%) and
murders (9%) (figure 1). The drowning and murder cases usually reflect requests
for dog teams. There has been an increase in the number of DAT searches and in
the percentage of total search load (table 1). The increase in the median age of
the U.S. population and an increased awareness of Alzheimer's disease are
believed to be responsible for this increase.17
The medical condition of the DAT subjects
after being found varied greatly. Eleven subjects (38%) required no medical
attention (class one) and were able to be escorted out of the woods. Twelve
subjects (41 %) required evacuation team (class two) (the forms do not always
state the specific medical problem). Six subjects (21 %) were found deceased
(class three). In searches for elderly subjects not suffering from DAT (n=10),
six subjects were classified as class one (60%); one subject was class two (10%)
due to hypothermia and dehydration; and three subjects (30% ) were found
deceased (heart attack, drowning, unrecorded). There is no relationship between
the age of the DAT subject and outcome (class) of the subject (figure 2).
See PDF Version
Figure 2: Class vs. Age.
Twenty-five (25) of the DAT searches have
data on the subject's distance from the point last seen (PLS) .In all 29
searches, the subject was located by either the search effort or by others. The
four missing data points represent a failure to complete the data form
correctly. The mean distance from the PLS is 0.6 miles (1.0 km). The median
distance is 0.5 miles (0.8 km) with a range of 0-2 miles (0-1.2 km) (figure 3).
This can be compared to elderly cases without DAT where a mean distance from the
PLS is 2.3 miles (3.8 km). The median distance is 2.5 miles (4.2 km) with a
range of 0.1-5 miles (0.2-8.3 km) (table 2). There is no relationship between
the DAT subject's age and distance from the PLS (figure 4). However, there is a
positive relationship between the distance from the PLS and subject class
(figure 5).
Most DAT subjects are last seen at either
their own residence or a nursing home (table 3) .In addition, the five subjects
spotted on a road initially departed from a nursing home or residence. The
terrain the subject was located in was recorded in 24 cases. The majority of
subjects are found in drainages/creeks or heavy brush/briars (table 4). With
the three cases found in a house, two were found hiding in their own house and
one traveled to a previous residence. In most searches, the subject is found
wandering by nonsearchers and not by search teams. In many cases, the subject is
located before trained searchers arrive on the scene. Sweep teams are the most
successful search technique (table 5).
DAT subjects requiring evacuation (n=10)
suffered from hypothermia (56%) and/or dehydration (44%). No hospital records
were reviewed to support the field diagnosis. Deceased subjects (n=6) appeared
to have succumbed to hypothermia ( 4) , drowned (2) or died from heart disease
(1). Physical clues were located in only three searches (14%). These included
broken branches leading to the subject's shoe, sugar packets taken from a
cafeteria and a personal letter. Fourteen subjects walked across a road
(67%), three subjects entered the woods after walking on a road (14%
) and four subjects did not cross any roads (19% ).
Discussion
The data characterizing
missing persons as suffering from DAT was provided by caregivers during the
investigative component of the search. Investigators within Virginia are
suspicious of the potential of DAT in elderly subjects. The Lost Person
Questionnaire, a standard data collection tool used on all state searches,
prompts the investigator to pursue mental alterations. While several other
conditions can cause dementia and therefore be confused with Alzheimer's
disease, this has minimal impact on the usefulness of the collected data. During
searches (by definition the subject is not present), a definitive classification
as DAT is impossible unless previously made by a physician. This is particularly
true of subjects who become lost in wilderness and rural settings who often
belong to a lower socioeconomic group and receive less healthcare. 18
Therefore, search managers will almost always be unable to differentiate
between dementia and DAT. If the predictive database (this study) potentially
includes both groups, then this dilemma is controlled.
The data allows the development of a DAT
subject profile. The subject usually disappears from their residence or nursing
home. While not documented in this study, it is worth noting that it has been
the principal investigator's personal observation on 25 searches for DAT
subjects that almost all had become lost before. Generally, the family or local
authorities had been able to locate the subject rapidly. This tendency to become
lost is consistent with an increasing tendency to wander. Once the subjects
become lost they are generally found close to the PLS. While the investigators
have heard numerous reports of Alzheimer's subjects walking great distances
(10-15 miles), no such case appeared in the Virginia caseload. As a larger data
pool develops, the mean distance of 0.6 miles will almost certainly increase.
However, the median distance of 0.5 miles may remain stable.
It is unknown if the subjects spend
considerable time wandering or if they walk a fairly direct path. Following a
path of least resistance is supported by the considerable number (63%) of DAT
subjects found in drainage/creeks or brush/briars. This indicates they walked
downhill. Another 25% of the subjects appear to have become stuck in thick brush
or briars (a feature untrained searchers often avoid) .Both terrain features
indicate a scenario of the subject traveling a path of least resistance until
they reach a creek or get stuck in briars.
Based on the authors' personal search
notes, subjects are often found a short distance off a road or other feature
that is easily traveled. The possibility of following a direct path is also
supported by a number of subjects who were found between the PLS and a target
location (favorite place, former residence, etc.). A line drawn from the
subject's residence and the PLS (if a later sighting occurred) often predicts
where the subject can be found. The difficulty the incident commander faces is
determining the potential target.
The age of the subject has no predictive
value for the subject's survivability or distance found from the PLS. It would
be worthwhile to investigate the relationship between the severity of DAT (mild,
moderate, severe) with search outcome and distance found from the PLS. The
relationship between the survivability of the subject and distance from the PLS
can be easily explained. The search area and time required to find the subject
grows exponentially as the radius increases. The longer the subject is exposed
to the elements, the less their chance of survival. This relationship has little
operational use since during a search the distance the subject is from the PLS
is unknown.
Unfortunately, the data forms do not
consistently provide information about the exact medical condition of the
subject when found. If the subject was found deceased, the incident commander
did not receive a copy of the autopsy or the autopsy did not specify the exact
cause of death. In those subjects requiring evacuation, making a field diagnosis
is often difficult. However, none of the data forms report trauma. The only
indicated disorders included hypothermia, dehydration, drowning, heart disease
and unknown. Therefore, it appears DA T subjects are most likely to succumb
to the environment and not to any injuries or pre-existing
diseases
Figure 4: Age vs. Distance Found From PLS.
Figure 5: Class vs. Distance Found From PLS.
See PDF Version
.
An important aspect in redeploying
resources and ultimately finding the missing subject is looking for and
analyzing clues. The large percentage of searches without clues (86%) is most
likely due to the fact that DAT subjects have no equipment, food or extra
clothes to discard. Almost all subjects are found with all their clothes, so
very few personal clues exist for searchers to find. All of the clues located
required an active investigation to verify and determine their value. Resources
capable of locating scent (dog teams) or passage (trackers) may playa critical
role in locating the subjects.
Containment plays a small role in
locating DAT subjects. Periodic road patrols still have value due to the number
of DAT subjects located on roads (8%). Indeed, within Virginia the technique is
seldom used. It is clear that DAT subjects will cross roads. In many of these
cases the subject crossed two lane paved roads that are heavily traveled.
To better predict DAT missing subject
behavior, a much larger pool of data is required. It is important to
recognize the critical role that local terrain may have in distances covered.
Virginia consists of a swampy tidewater region, rolling hills in a piedmont
region and a heavily forested mountainous region. Numerous roads and paths
crisscross most wilderness regions. An obvious need to better document the
observations that DAT subjects have wandered previously, aimed for some target,
crossed roads, generally traveled downhill, usually traveled only a short
distance, easily bec4Ine stuck in briars and easily died or succumbed to
environmental disorders must be pursued in larger national prospective studies.
Summary
Table 3 Point Last Seen
(PLS)
Personal
Home |
9
(36%) |
Nursing
Home |
9
(36%) |
Roadway |
5
(20%) |
Relatives |
2
(8%) |
Creeks/Drainages |
9
(38%) |
Bushes/Briars |
6
(25%) |
Open
Field |
4
(17%) |
Roadway |
3
(12%) |
House |
2
(8%) |
Table 5: Successful Field
Techniques
Non
Searchers |
10
(42%) |
Sweep |
6
(25%) |
Scratch
(Hasty) |
3
(13%) |
Air Scent
Dog |
3
(13%) |
Helicopter |
2
(8%) |
Suggested Search Techniques
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Winnie Pennington
and Ralph Wilfong of the Virginia Department of Emergency Services for supplying
copies of state mission records.
End Notes
1. Syrotuck, W.1973. A statistical
analysis of lost persons in wilderness areas. Westmoreland, NY: Arner
Publications.
2. Mitchell, B. 1986. A
summary of the National Association for Search and Rescue data collection and
analysis program for 1980- 1985. Fairfax, VA: NASAR.
3. NASAR. 1987.
Managing the search function. Fairfax, V A: NASAR.
4. NASAR. 1987.
Incident commander field hand- book: Search and rescue. Fairfax, V A:
NASAR.
5. Gruetzner, H. 1988.
Alzheimer's-A caregivers guide and sourcebook. New York: John Wiley &
Sons, Inc.
6. Evans, D., et al.
1989. Prevalence of Alzheimer's disease in a community population of older persons.JAMA 262:2551-6.
7. Folstein, M.R.;
Folstein, S.; and McHugh, P.R. 1975. Mini-mental state: A practical method for
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. ] Psychiatric Res.
12:189.
8. Blessed, G.;
Tomlinson, B.; and Roth, M. 1968. The association between quantitative methods
of dementia and senile change in the cerebral gray matter of elderly subjects.
Br.] Psychiatry 114:797.
9. American Psychiatric
Association. 1980. Diagnostic and statistical manual-III. Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Press.
10. Kahn, R., e\ al.
1960. A brief objective measure for the determination of mental status in the
aged. Am. ] Psychiatry 117:326- 328.
11. Reifler, B. 1983.
Clinical aspects of Alzheimer's disease. Modem Med. Canada 38:17-20.
12. Teri, L.; Larson, E.;
and Reifler, B. 1988. Behavioral disturbance in dementia of the Alzheimer's
type.] American Geriatrics Society 36:1-6.
13. Reifler, B., et al.
1981. Treatment results at a multispecialty clinic for impaired elderly and
their families.] Am. Geriatric Soc. 29:279.
14. Reifler, B.; Larson,
E.; and Hanley, R. 1982. Coexisting cognitive impairment and depression in
geriatric outpatients. Am.] Psychiatry 139:623.
15. Adams, R., and
Victor, M. 1989. Principles of neurology, 4th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.
16. Wilfong, R. 1987.
Time frame for survival-minutes and counting-Virginia enhances search and rescue
capabilities. Lifeline 11:2:10-13.
17. CDC Editorial Note.
1990. Mortalit;y patterns-United
States-1987. MMWR , 39:193-6.
18. Rosenblatt, RA. 1991.
The potential of the academic medical center to shape policy-Qriented rural
health research. Acad. Med. 66:662-7.
Robert J Koester has an MS in neurobiology from the University
of Virginia and is currently a member of the faculty in the Division of Natural
Science. He is the chairman of the Appalachian Search and Rescue Conference,
where he has been a member for 11 years. Koester has served as incident
commander on 70 search sites and participated in 25 searches for
Alzheimer's subjects. David E. Stooksbury is with the Department of
Environmental Sciences' Virginia State Climatology Office at the University of
Virginia. He is also a member of the Appalachian Search and Rescue Conference.
Citation: Koester, R. & Stooksbury, D
(1992) "Lost Alzheimer's Subjects-Profiles and Statistics" Response
11:4:20-26